Solo Episode
September 12, 2021

ADHD & How Anyone Can Improve Their Focus

Listen or watch on your favorite platforms

In this episode, Dr. Huberman discusses ADHD (Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder): what it is, the common myths, and the biology and psychology of ADHD.

He discusses both behavioral and pharmacologic treatments for ADHD and brain-machine interface tools. Dr. Huberman also discusses behavioral training protocols that can improve focus in people with ADHD and those without ADHD and for people of different ages. He discusses the role of dopamine in coordinating ‘default-mode’ and ‘task-related’ neural networks, attentional “blinks” (lapses of attention) and how to overcome them, and the role of actual blinks in time perception and attention. Finally, Dr. Huberman reviews some of the prescription and over-the-counter compounds for increasing focus, such as Adderall, Ritalin, Modafinil and Armodafinil, the racetams, Alpha-GPC and phosphatidylserine and the role of diet for managing ADHD (and the controversies of diet for ADHD).

The role of cell phones/technology in ADHD and ADHD-like challenges with focus are also discussed. Throughout, both basic science and clinical scenarios, as well as applicable tools and resources, are covered.

Links

No items found.
  • 00:00:00 Introduction & Note About Diagnosis
  • 00:03:27 Sponsors
  • 00:07:56 ADHD vs. ADD: Genetics, IQ, Rates in Kids & Adults
  • 00:13:00 Attention & Focus, Impulse Control
  • 00:14:57 Hyper-focus
  • 00:16:45 Time Perception
  • 00:18:25 The Pile System
  • 00:20:00 Working Memory
  • 00:24:10 Hyper-Focus & Dopamine
  • 00:26:40 Neural Circuits In ADHD: Default Mode Network & Task-Related Networks
  • 00:32:57 Low Dopamine in ADHD & Stimulant Use & Abuse
  • 00:37:10 Sugar, Ritalin, Adderall, Modafinil & Armodafinil
  • 00:47:00 Non-Prescribed Adderall, Caffeine, Nicotine
  • 00:49:18 How Stimulants “Teach” the Brains of ADHD Children to Focus
  • 00:52:00 When To Medicate: A Highly Informed (Anecdotal) Case Study
  • 00:56:35 Elimination Diets & Allergies In ADHD
  • 01:04:46 Omega-3 Fatty Acids: EPAs & DHAs
  • 01:07:00 Modulation vs Mediation of Biological Processes
  • 01:10:50 Attentional Blinks
  • 01:16:56 Open Monitoring & 17minute Focus Enhancement
  • 01:22:50 Blinking, Dopamine & Time Perception; & Focus Training
  • 01:30:10 Reverberatory Neural & Physical Activity
  • 01:33:40 Adderrall, Ritalin & Blink Frequency
  • 01:35:00 Cannabis
  • 01:37:30 Interoceptive Awareness
  • 01:41:15 Ritalin, Adderall, Modafinil, Armodafinil; Smart Drugs & Caffeine: Dangers
  • 01:48:05 DHA Fatty Acids, Phosphatidylserine
  • 0:1:50:54 Ginko Biloba
  • 01:51:45 Modafinil & Armodafanil: Dopamine Action & Orexin
  • 01:56:19 Acetylcholine: Circuits Underlying Focus; Alpha-GPC
  • 01:59:04 L-Tyrosine, (PEA) Phenylethylamine
  • 02:01:23 Racetams, Noopept
  • 02:05:15 Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation; Combining Technology & Pharmacology
  • 02:09:14 Smart Phones & ADHD & Sub-Clinical Focus Issues In Adults & Kids
  • 02:14:30 Synthesis/Summary
  • 02:16:10 Support for Podcast & Research, Supplement Resources

Become a Huberman Lab Premium member to access full episode transcripts & more

Members also get to submit questions for AMA episodes, plus access to exclusive bonus content. A significant portion of proceeds are donated to fund human scientific research.

Become a Member

or sign in to view this Transcript

Andrew Huberman:

Welcome to the Huberman Lab podcast, where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life.

Andrew Huberman:

I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. Today, we are going to talk all about attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, or ADHD. We are also going to talk about normal levels of focus, what are normal levels of focus, and how all of us, whether or not we have ADHD or not, can improve our ability to focus, our ability to rule out distraction — it turns out those are two separate things — as well as remember information better. We are also going to talk about how we can learn to relax while focusing, which turns out to be a critical component of learning new information and for coming up with new creative ideas.

Andrew Huberman:

So whether or not you have ADHD or know someone who does, or if you're somebody who feels that they do not have ADHD, but would simply like to improve their ability to focus or to be more creative, this episode is definitely for you as well. We are going to talk about drug-based tools that are out there. We are going to talk about behavioral tools. We will talk about the role of diet and supplementation, and we will talk about new emerging brain machine interface devices, things like transcranial magnetic stimulation. If you don't know what that is, don't worry, I will explain it to you. These are noninvasive methods for rewiring your brain in order to make focusing more natural for you, and to teach you how to increase your depth of focus.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, just a quick reminder that anytime we discuss a psychiatric disorder, it's important that we remember that all of us have the temptation to self-diagnose or to diagnose others. So as I list off some of the symptomology of ADHD, some of that symptomology might resonate with you. You might think, "Oh, maybe I have ADHD," or you might decide that someone definitely has ADHD. However, it is very important that you don't self-diagnose or diagnose somebody else. The clear and real diagnosis of ADHD really should be carried out by a psychiatrist, a physician or a very well-trained clinical psychologist.

Andrew Huberman:

There are clear criteria for what constitutes full-blown ADHD. However, many of us have constellations of symptoms that make us somewhat like somebody with ADHD. If you're struggling with focus nowadays, as a lot of people are because of stress, because of smartphone use, which turns out can induce adult ADHD — we'll talk about that — well, then pay attention to the symptomology. You may actually require professional treatment. You might not. Equally important is to remember that some of the terms that we cover, like impulse control and attention and concentration, are somewhat subjective, and they can change over time.

Andrew Huberman:

Some times we have a better level of attention than others. Maybe it depends on how we slept, or other events going on in our life, or something that we're entirely unaware of. The important thing to remember is that we can all improve our attentional capacity. We can all rewire the circuits that make heightened levels of focus more accessible to us. We can do that through multiple types of interventions, and we are going to cover all those interventions today.

Andrew Huberman:

Before we march into the material, I'd like to remind that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. It is however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero-cost-to-consumer information about science and science-related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. Let's talk about ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Let's also talk about focus and attention, and everybody's ability to focus and attend, not just people with ADHD.

Andrew Huberman:

We are also going to talk about tools that would allow anyone, whether or not they have ADHD or not, to enhance their level of concentration and focus. Now, ADHD used to be called ADD, attention deficit disorder. We have record of ADD in the medical literature dating back to as early as 1904. Now, there's nothing special about 1904. That's just the first time that it showed up in the standard medical literature. We have to believe that ADD, which we now call ADHD, existed before 1904, and probably long before 1904. Why? Well, because it has a strong genetic component.

Andrew Huberman:

If you have a close relative that has ADHD, there's a much higher probability that you will have ADHD, and that probability goes up depending on how closely related to that person you happen to be. So for instance, if you're an identical twin, and your twin has ADHD, there's a very high concordance, as we say, a very high probability that you will have ADHD, up to 75% chance. If you have a fraternal twin with ADHD, that number goes down a bit, in the 50% to 60% range, and so on. If you have a parent with ADHD, that number ranges anywhere from 10% to 25% likelihood that you will have ADHD. If you have two parents, and so on and so on; so there's a genetic component.

Andrew Huberman:

That genetic component, it turns out, relates directly to how specific neural circuits in the brain wire up, the chemicals they use and the way they use those chemicals — a topic that we are going to discuss in depth today. Now, if you have a close relative with ADHD, that does not mean that you are fated to have ADHD. If you happen to have ADHD, there are ways to overcome those symptoms of lack of attention, impulsivity and so on. Another important point about ADHD is that it has nothing to do with intelligence, whether or not we're talking about intelligence measured by a standard IQ test, a rather controversial issue.

Andrew Huberman:

As many of you probably know, there are lots of forms of intelligence that a standard IQ test just wouldn't pick up. Emotional intelligence, musical intelligence, spatial intelligence, all sorts of intelligences, none of them are related to ADHD. Being very high functioning doesn't make you more likely to have ADHD, and being ADHD doesn't necessarily mean that you have a low IQ. There are people with ADHD who have low IQs, people with ADHD with high IQs, people with ADHD with high emotional IQ or with low IQ in the emotional scale. It's all over the place. The important point is that your ability to attend and focus does not relate to how smart you are or your IQ of any type, not just a standard IQ.

Andrew Huberman:

The renaming of ADD to ADHD took place in the mid to late 1980s, when the psychiatric community and the psychological community started taking better notice of the fact that so-called hyperactive kids also had attentional issues. This might seem obvious, but there's been extensive and ongoing revision of the criteria for designating a psychiatric disorder, and this is still an ongoing process even today. So in the mid '80s, we started hearing about ADHD, and then gradually that term ADD has been dropped away. However, just the renaming of ADD to ADHD has led to much better diagnosis and detection of ADHD.

Andrew Huberman:

So right now, the current estimates are that about one in 10 children, and probably more, have ADHD. The current estimates are anywhere from 10%, one in 10, to as high as 12%. Now, fortunately, about half of those will resolve with proper treatment, but the other half typically don't. The other thing that we are seeing a lot nowadays is increased levels of ADHD in adults. There are some question as to whether or not those adults had ADHD that went undetected during their childhood, or whether or not ADHD is now cropping up in adulthood due to the way that we are interacting with the world.

Andrew Huberman:

In particular, smart phone use, the combination of email, text, real-world interactions, multiple apps and streams of media and social media all coming in at once, trying to manage life, all of the things that are going on are creating a cloud of pools on our attention. So, there is this question to whether or not we are creating ADHD in adults that never had ADHD prior to being an adult. Let's talk about attention. First, let's just define what we mean by attention. Out there in the scientific literature and in discussions about ADHD, we will hear things like attention and focus and concentration and impulse control.

Andrew Huberman:

For sake of today's discussion, attention, focus and concentration are essentially the same thing. We could split hairs, and the scientific literature does split hairs about these. But if we want to understand the biology, and we want to have a straightforward conversation about ADHD, if I say attention or focus, I'm basically referring to the same thing, unless I specify otherwise. So, people with ADHD have trouble holding their attention. What is attention? Well, attention is perception. It's how we are perceiving the sensory world. So just a little bit of Neurobiology 101: we are sensing things all the time.

Andrew Huberman:

There's information coming into our nervous system all the time. For instance, right now, you are hearing sound waves. You are seeing things. You are sensing things against your skin, but you are only paying attention to some of those, and the ones that you're paying attention to are your perceptions. So if you hear my voice, you are perceiving my voice. You are not paying attention to your other senses at the moment, okay? You might even be outside in a breeze, and until I said that, you might not be perceiving that breeze, but your body was sensing it all along.

Andrew Huberman:

Attention and focus are more or less the same thing, but impulse control is something separate, because impulse control requires pushing out or putting the blinders on to sensory events in our environment. It means lack of perception. Impulse control is about limiting our perception. People with ADHD have poor attention, and they have high levels of impulsivity. They're easily distractible, but the way that shows up is very surprising. You might think that people with ADHD just simply can't attend to anything, they really can't focus even if they really want to, but that's simply not the case.

Andrew Huberman:

People with ADHD, yes, they are distractible. Yes, they are impulsive. Yes, they are easily annoyed by things happening in the room. They sometimes have a high level of emotionality as well, not always, but often. However, people with ADHD can have a hyperfocus, an incredible ability to focus on things that they really enjoy or are intrigued by. Now, this is a very important point, because typically we think of somebody with ADHD as being really wild and hyperactive or having no ability whatsoever to sit still and attend.

Andrew Huberman:

While that phenotype, as we call it, that contour of behavior and cognition, can exist, many people, if not all people with ADHD, if you give them something they really love, like if the child loves video games, or if a child loves to draw, or if an adult loves a particular type of movie or a person very much, they will obtain laser focus without any effort. That tells us that people with ADHD have the capacity to attend, but they can't engage that attention for things that they don't really, really want to do. As we all know, much of life, whether or not you're a child or an adult, involves doing a lot of things that we don't want to do.

Andrew Huberman:

Much of our schooling involves doing things that we would prefer not to do and forcing ourselves to do it, to attend even though we are not super interested in what we are attending to. There are a couple other things that people with ADHD display quite often. One is challenges with time perception. Now, time perception is a fascinating aspect of how our brain works. Later, we're going to talk about time perception and how you can actually get better at time perception. It's very likely that right now you are doing things that get in the way of optimal time perception, and I will tell you how to adjust your ability to measure time with your brain.

Andrew Huberman:

People with ADHD often run late. They often procrastinate. But what's interesting and surprising is that if they are given a deadline, they actually can perceive time very well. They often can focus very well if the consequences of not completing a task or not attending are severe enough. It's a little bit like the way that people with ADHD can really focus if they like something. Well, if they're scared enough about the consequences of not attending, oftentimes, not always, but oftentimes they can attend. If they're not really concerned about a deadline or a consequence, well, then they tend to lose track of time, and they tend to underestimate how long things will take.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, many people do that, not just people with ADHD, but people with ADHD have challenges understanding how to line up the activities of their day in order to meet particular deadlines, even if it's just a simple thing like finishing one set of tasks before lunch. Oftentimes, they will remember that lunch starts at noon, but somehow, they aren't able to fill the intervening time in a way that's productive, and they can obsess about the upcoming deadline, for instance. We will talk about how to remedy this. In addition, their spatial organization skills are often subpar. Not always, but often, you will find that somebody with ADHD uses what's called the pile system in order to organize things.

Andrew Huberman:

They will take many belongings, and this could be in the kitchen or in their bedroom or in their office or in any space, and they will start piling things up according to a categorization system that makes sense to them and only them. It doesn't really have any logical framework. Now, many people use the pile system. And if you use the pile system, that doesn't mean that you have ADHD. In fact, if you're unpacking a house, or you've moved recently, or you've received a lot of presents recently, the pile system makes perfect sense to organize your space.

Andrew Huberman:

But people with ADHD tend to organize things according to the pile system all the time, and that pile system doesn't work for them. That's the key distinction, that they use a filing system, and it's not really files, they're piling things up in a way that makes sense to them, but then it doesn't work for them in terms of what task they actually need to perform. They can't find things, or if anyone moves one thing, then it's very disruptive to their overall plan because their overall plan doesn't really work in the first place. That's a common phenotype as we call it.

Andrew Huberman:

A phenotype, by the way, is just an expression of a particular set of underlying genetic or psychological components, so we say the phenotype. A phenotype could be brown hair and green eyes. For me, a phenotype could also be that somebody uses the piling system. The other thing that people with ADHD have real trouble with is so-called working memory. Now, you might think that people with ADHD would have really poor memories, but in fact, that's not the case. People with ADHD often can have a terrific memory for past events. They can remember upcoming events quite well. Their memory is clearly working.

Andrew Huberman:

However, one aspect of memory, in particular, that we call working memory, is often disrupted. Working memory is the ability to keep specific information online to recycle it in your brain over and over again so that you can use it in the immediate or short term. A good example of this would be you meet somebody. They tell you their name. They give you their phone number verbally, and you have to walk back to your phone, and enter it into your phone. People without ADHD might have to put some effort into it. It might feel like a bit of a struggle, but typically, they would be able to recite that phone number in their mind over and over, and then put it into their phone.

Andrew Huberman:

People with ADHD tend to lose the ability, or lack the ability to remember things that they just need to keep online for anywhere from 10 seconds to a minute or two. So a string of numbers like 643-781, for most people, would be pretty easy, 643-781, 643-781. You could probably remember that a minute from now without writing it down. But if you add one more number to that 643-7813, it gets tougher. There's a reason why phone numbers typically have seven digits in them. Of course, there's an area code. But remembering information that strings out longer than seven numbers or a sentence or two, that's challenging for most people. People with ADHD have severe challenges, even with much smaller batches of information over even much smaller batches of time.

Andrew Huberman:

Deficits in working memory are also something that we see in people who have frontal temporal dementia, so damage to the frontal lobes, or age-related cognitive decline. It will come as no surprise that later when we discuss treatments, supplements and other tools for ADHD, that many of those treatments, supplements and tools for ADHD are similar to the ones that work for age-related cognitive decline. We've more or less established a menu of items that people with ADHD tend to have. Some have all of them. Some have just a subset of them.

Andrew Huberman:

Their severity can range from very intense to mild. But in general, it's challenges with attention and focus, challenges with impulse control. They get annoyed easily. They have an impulsivity. They can't stay on task. Time perception can be off. They use the piling system or a system that doesn't work well for them in order to organize their things in physical space, and they have a hard time with anything that's mundane that they're not really interested in. But again, I just want to highlight that people with ADHD are able to obtain heightened levels of focus, even hyperfocus, for things that are exciting to them and that they really want to engage in.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, you have the contour of what ADHD is. If you're somebody who doesn't have ADHD, you should also be asking yourself, "Which aspects of ADHD are similar to things I've experienced before?" Because what we know about the healthy brain is that there's also a range of abilities to focus. Some people focus very well on any task. You give them a task. They can just laser in on that task. Other people, they have to fight an internal battle. They have to convince themselves that it's important or interesting. They have to incentivize themselves internally.

Andrew Huberman:

Other people — doesn't matter. They could be bored to tears with the information, but they can do it just because they are "very disciplined people." We tend to admire those people. But as you'll see a little bit later, it's not clear that that's the best way to run your attentional system. There might be something to this business of having heightened levels of attention for the things that you are most interested or excited by. Let's drill into this issue of why people with ADHD actually can focus very intensely on things that they enjoy and are curious about.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, enjoyment and curiosity are psychological terms. They're not even really psychological terms. They're just the way that we describe our human experience of liking things, wanting to know more about them. But from a neurobiological perspective, they have a very clear identity and signature, and that's dopamine. Dopamine is released from neurons. It's what we call a neuromodulator. As a neuromodulator, it changes the activity of the circuits in the brain such that certain circuits are more active than others.

Andrew Huberman:

In particular, dopamine creates a heightened state of focus. It tends to contract our visual world, and it tends to make us pay attention to things that are outside and beyond the confines of our skin. That's what we call exteroception. Dopamine also tends to put us in a state of motivation and wanting things outside the confines of our skin. So whether or not we're pursuing something physical in our world, or whether or not we're pursuing information in our outside world, dopamine is largely responsible for our ability and our drive to do that, but dopamine, as a neuromodulator, is also involved in changing the way that we perceive the world.

Andrew Huberman:

So as I mentioned earlier, you have all these senses coming in, and you can only perceive some of them because you're only paying attention to some of them. Dopamine, when it's released in our brain, tends to turn on areas of our brain that narrow our visual focus and our auditory focus. It creates a cone of auditory attention that's very narrow. It creates a tunnel of visual attention that's very narrow. Whereas when we have less dopamine, we tend to view the entire world. We tend to see the whole scene that we are in. We tend to hear everything all at once.

Andrew Huberman:

So as I describe this, hopefully you're already starting to see and understand how having dopamine release can allow a person, whether or not they have ADHD or not, to direct their attention to particular things in their environment. Now, what we're doing is we're moving away from attention as this vague, ambiguous term, and we're giving it a neurochemical identity, dopamine. We are giving it a neural circuit identity. Just to put a little bit of flavor and detail on which neural circuits those are, I wanted to discuss two general types of neural circuits that dopamine tends to enhance.

Andrew Huberman:

Let's talk neural circuits. For those of you that love hearing neuroscience nomenclature, you're going to eat this part up. For those of you that don't like a lot of names of brain areas, I invite you to tune out or just try and grab the top contour of this. I will describe it in pretty general terms, but I will give some detail, because I know there are some of you out there who really want to dig deeper into what the exact structures and connectivities are. There are two main types of circuits that we need to think about with respect to ADHD, attention, and dopamine. The first one is called the default mode network.

Andrew Huberman:

The default mode network is the network of brain areas in your brain, in my brain, and in everybody's brain that is active when we're not doing anything, when we're just sitting there idle at rest. Now, it's very hard to not think about anything, but when you're not engaged in any type of specific task, so you're not driving. You're not playing a video game. You're not trying to study. You're not trying to listen. You're just sitting there letting your brain go wherever it wants to go. Your default mode network underlies that state of mind.

Andrew Huberman:

The other set of circuits that we're going to think about and talk about with respect to ADHD are the task networks, the networks of the brain that make you goal oriented, or that are at least trying to make you goal oriented. Those are a completely different set of brain areas. However, the default mode network and these task networks are communicating with one another, and they're doing that in very interesting ways. So first, I want to describe how these two sets of brain areas, the default mode network and the task networks, normally interact. A little bit of naming here. Again, feel free to ignore it if you don't want this level of detail, but the default mode network includes an area called the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Andrew Huberman:

Frontal cortex, no surprise, is in the front. You have a dorsal, the top, and side, lateral part, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. You got one on each side of your brain. Then you have a brain area called the posterior cingulate cortex. Then you have an area called the lateral parietal lobe. Again, you don't need to remember these names, but these are three brain areas that normally are synchronized in their activity. So when one of these areas is active in a typical person, the other areas would be active as well. So, it's a little bit like a symphony or a band, a three-piece band.

Andrew Huberman:

It's like drums, guitar and bass. They're playing together. That's how it is in a typical person. A person with ADHD, or even a person who has subclinical ADHD, or in any human being who hasn't slept well, what you find is the default mode network is not synchronized. These brain areas are just not playing well together. Now, the task networks include a different set of structures. It still involves the prefrontal cortex, but it's a different part of the prefrontal cortex. It tends to be the medial prefrontal cortex. There are some other brain areas that the medial prefrontal cortex is communicating to all the time, mainly to suppress impulses.

Andrew Huberman:

It's shutting down the desire to stand up or to scratch the side of your cheek or your nose if you're trying not to do that. Anytime you're restricting your behavior, these task-directed networks are very active. Now, normally in a person without ADHD, the task networks and the default mode networks are going in seesaw fashion. They are actually what we call anticorrelated. It's not just that they are not correlated, they are actually opposing one another. They are anticorrelated. In a person with ADHD, the default mode networks and the task networks are actually more coordinated.

Andrew Huberman:

That might come as surprising. I think that we all have this tendency to jump to conclusion and assume that somebody who doesn't have an easy time paying attention, or has ADHD, that their brain must be completely incoherent, that it's not working well, because everything's out of whack. But there's something interesting about people with ADHD whereby the task networks and the default mode networks are actually working together in a way that's correlated, and that is what's abnormal. This would be like the guitar, bass and the drums playing together in a way where the bass isn't keeping the back beat, and the drums aren't keeping the back beat that they're playing together.

Andrew Huberman:

They're all playing the melodies and harmonies in a way that just doesn't sound right. That's what's going on in the brain of somebody with ADHD. We can now confidently say, based on brain imaging studies, that when somebody gets better when they're treated for ADHD, or when they age out of ADHD, as sometimes is the case, that the default mode networks and the task networks tend to become anticorrelated again. That's the underlying neurobiology, but you'll notice that I didn't mention dopamine at all. What dopamine is doing in this context is dopamine is acting like a conductor. Dopamine is saying, "This circuit should be active, then that circuit should be active. It should be default mode network, and then when the default mode network is not active, then it should be the task network," so it's really acting as a conductor, saying, "You go now. You go now. You go now. You go." In ADHD, there's something about the dopamine system that is not allowing it to conduct these networks and make sure that they stay, what the engineers or physicists or mathematicians would say, out of phase, to be anticorrelated. Out of phase and anticorrelated, essentially the same thing, at least for purposes of this discussion.

Andrew Huberman:

That raises two questions. Could it be that dopamine is not at sufficiently high levels, or could it be that dopamine is just doing it all wrong? In other words, is there no conductor or is the conductor playing with, like, little tiny toothpicks, and so the instruments can't see what they're supposed to do? They can't get the instruction, because it's just not loud enough so to speak. Could it be that the information is getting out, but the information that's getting out is wrong? The conductor is there, but the conductor isn't very good at conducting.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, we can gain insight into how this system works and fails, and how to treat it, by looking at some of the current and previous treatments for ADHD, as well as some of the recreational drugs that people with ADHD tend to pursue and like. Now, I'm certainly not a proponent of people with ADHD taking drugs recreationally. That's not what this is about. But if you look at their drug-seeking behavior, and you couple that drug-seeking behavior to their desire to remedy their attention deficit, you start gaining some really interesting insight into how dopamine is regulating these circuits in normal circumstances and in people with ADHD.

Andrew Huberman:

What exactly is going on with the dopamine system in people with ADHD, and what's going on with the dopamine system in people that have terrific levels of attention for any task? Well, in the year 2015, an important paper came out. The first author is Spencer. It came out in a journal called Biological Psychiatry. It formalized the so-called low dopamine hypothesis of ADHD. The idea that dopamine was somehow involved or not at the appropriate levels in people with ADHD had been around for a pretty long time. But, a formal proposition of the low dopamine hypothesis led to some really important experiments and understanding of what goes wrong in ADHD.

Andrew Huberman:

It turns out that if dopamine levels are too low, in particular circuits in the brain, that it leads to unnecessary firing of neurons in the brain that are unrelated to the task that one is trying to do, and that is unrelated to the information that one is trying to focus on. So if you think back before, you've got this default mode network and a task-related network, and they need to be in this concert of anticorrelation. In ADHD, they're firing together. Well, the problem seems to be that when dopamine is low, certain neurons are firing when they shouldn't be. This is like a band, right? We'll go back to our band. That's a guitar, a bass and a person playing the drums. It's as if one of those, or several of those instruments are playing notes when they shouldn't be playing, right?

Andrew Huberman:

The pauses in music are just as important as the actual playing of notes. When dopamine is too low, neurons fire more than they should in these networks that govern attention. This is the so-called low dopamine hypothesis. If you start looking anecdotally at what people with ADHD have done for decades, not just recently since the low dopamine hypothesis has been proposed, but what they were doing in the 1950s and in the 1940s and in the 1960s, what you find is that they tend to use recreational drugs or they tend to indulge in nondrug stimulants, so things like drinking six cups of coffee, or quadruple espressos, or when it was more prominent, smoking a half a pack of cigarettes and drinking four cups of coffee a day, or if the person had access to it, using cocaine as a recreational drug, or amphetamine as a recreational drug.

Andrew Huberman:

All of those substances that I just described, in particular cocaine and amphetamine, but also coffee and cigarettes, increase levels of multiple neurotransmitters, but all have the quality of increasing levels of dopamine in the brain, and in particular in the regions of the brain that regulate attention, and these task-related and default mode networks. Now, young children, fortunately, don't have access to those kinds of stimulants most of the time. Those stimulants all have high potential for abuse in adults, so we will talk about the potential for abuse in a few minutes. But if you look at children, even very young children with ADHD, they show things like preference for sugary foods, which also act as dopamine-inducing stimulants.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, of course, once they get access to soda pop and coffee and tea, they start to indulge in those more than other people. For a long time, it was thought that children with ADHD consumed too many sugary foods, or drank too much soda, or adults with ADHD would take recreational drugs like methamphetamine or cocaine, or would drink coffee to excess or smoke cigarettes to excess, because they had poor levels of attention, and because they couldn't make good decisions. They were too impulsive, and so forth.

Andrew Huberman:

While that certainly could be the case, knowing what we now know about dopamine and the fact that having enough dopamine is required in order to coordinate these neural circuits that allow for focus and quality decision-making, an equally valid idea is that these children and these adults are actually trying to self-medicate by pursuing these compounds, right? Things like cocaine lead to huge increases in dopamine. Well, what happens when somebody with ADHD takes that drug? It turns out they actually obtain heightened levels of focus. Their ability to focus on things other than things they absolutely care intensely about goes up. Likewise, children who-

Andrew Huberman:

-about goes up. Likewise, children who consume anything that increases their levels of dopamine, if those children have ADHD, they tend to be calmer. They tend to be able to focus more. Now, this is very different than children who do not have ADHD. When they consume too much sugar, they tend to become super hyperactive. When they consume any kind of stimulant, they tend to go wild and run around like crazy. I actually have an anecdote about this just to illustrate it. I have a friend, he has two children that are now in their teens and twenties, but when they were little, one time I brought them some chocolate just as a gift when I showed up at their house, and within 30 minutes the kids were running around like crazy. I mean, they were pretty high energy kids, but they were going bonkers. And that's actually when the mother, my friend at the time and fortunately still now, looked at the chocolate and realized that it was chocolate with espresso beans in it. It was like dark chocolate espresso beans.

Andrew Huberman:

So I was really at fault there. You don't want to give kids dark chocolate with espresso beans, but what you're really seeing, that hyperactivity, that is dopamine. Okay? It's the sugar, combined with the caffeine, in this case, combined with a few other compounds that exist in chocolate that really increase our levels of alertness and our tendency to want to move around a lot. Okay. So, dopamine, and low levels of dopamine, apparently are what's wrong in people with ADHD. That dopamine hypothesis is what led to the idea that treating people, children and adults included, with dopaminergic compounds would somehow increase their ability to focus. And if you look at the major drugs that were developed and now marketed by pharmaceutical companies for the treatment of ADHD, those drugs have names like Ritalin. Nowadays. It's typically things like Adderall, modafinil, and some of the other derivatives.

Andrew Huberman:

They all serve to increase levels of dopamine, in particular, dopamine in the networks that control task-directed behavior, and that coordinate the default mode network and these task-related networks. So, many of you have probably heard of Ritalin. Ritalin is a prescription stimulant that is prescribed for ADHD, as well as for narcolepsy. Narcolepsy is a condition which people tend to fall asleep during the daytime quite a lot. It's excessive daytime sleepiness, not due to lack of sleep at night, but also tend to fall asleep when they get excited, if they're really emotionally excited or about to eat, or any other kind of activity that would normally get somebody really aroused and alert, people with narcolepsy tend to fall asleep or they tend to become what's called cataplectic. They tend to just sort of go limp in the muscles. So it's this invasion of sleep into the daytime.

Andrew Huberman:

It's dysregulated by emotion. You can imagine why a stimulant, something that would wake you up, make you very alert, focused and motivated would be a good treatment for narcolepsy. Adderall also is used to treat ADHD and to treat narcolepsy. Things like modafinil also used to treat ADHD and narcolepsy. So you're sensing a theme here. So what are the differences and similarities between these drugs, and what can that tell us about ADHD? Well, Ritalin was one of the first-generation drugs that was prescribed for ADHD in order to deal head on with this dopamine hypothesis, this idea that in ADHD, dopamine levels are too low. Nowadays, Adderall is the more typically prescribed drug for ADHD. That has to do with some of the so-called pharmacokinetics, the rate at which those drugs enter the system and how long they last in the system. So for instance, Ritalin was a drug that was packaged into various time-release formulas, whereas initially, Adderall was only released in a form that had a very short life.

Andrew Huberman:

So meaning that it wasn't in the bloodstream very long and didn't affect the brain for very long. And so the dosages could be controlled in a more typical way. Without going into a lot of tangential detail, as you all know, at different times of day, you tend to be more or less alert. So a long sustained-release drug, while that might sound like a really terrific thing, if that drug is having an effect of making you more alert and it's released across very many hours of your day, there might be periods of your day when you feel too alert, periods of your day when you feel just right, and periods of your day when you wished that you were more alert. These are some of the pharmacokinetics — kinetics, meaning movement — of the different compounds within the bloodstream and brain that you could imagine in a very real way would impact whether or not someone would feel really good on one of these drugs, or whether or not they would feel too anxious or too sleepy, and so on.

Andrew Huberman:

Let's take a step back for a second and just ask, what are these drugs? We know they increased dopamine, but what are they really? Well Ritalin, also called methylphenidate, is very similar to amphetamine — speed, or what's typically called speed in the street drug nomenclature. Adderall, which goes by various other names, so Adderall, Adderall XR, Mydayis, things like that. Adderall is basically a combination of amphetamine and dextroamphetamine. Now, some of you probably realize this, that Adderall is amphetamine, but I'm guessing that there are a good number of you out there, perhaps even parents and kids, that don't realize that these drugs like cocaine and amphetamine, methamphetamine, which are incredibly dangerous and incredibly habit-forming and have high potential for abuse, well, the pharmaceutical versions of those are exactly what are used to treat ADHD. Now, they're not exactly like cocaine or methamphetamine, but they are structurally and chemically very similar, and their net effect in the brain and body is essentially the same, which is to increase dopamine, primarily, but also to increase levels of a neuromodul

Andrew Huberman:

ator called epinephrine or norepinephrine, also called noradrenaline and adrenaline — those names are the same — and to some extent to increase levels of serotonin in the brain and blood, but not so much serotonin. That's just kind of a small smidgen of effect. Okay, so dopamine way up, norepinephrine and adrenaline way up. So that's motivation, drive, focus and energy, and to some extent a little bit of serotonin, which is really more about feeling calm and relaxed, and you could imagine why that would be a good balancing effect for dopamine and norepinephrine. So what I'm essentially saying is that the drugs that are used to treat ADHD are stimulants, and they look very much like, in fact nearly identical to, some of the so-called street drug stimulants that we all hear are so terrible. However, I do want to emphasize that at the appropriate dosages, and working with a quality psychiatrist or neurologist or family physician, does have to be a board-certified MD that prescribes these things, many people with ADHD achieve excellent relief with these drugs. Not all of them, but many of them do, especially if these treatments are started early in life. So now knowing what these drugs are, I want to raise the question of why prescribe these drugs? I mean, everyone has to make a decision for themselves or for their child as to whether or not they're going to take these things or not. I also want to acknowledge that many people out there, many, many people out there, are taking these drugs even though they have not been clinically diagnosed with ADHD. When I say these drugs, I'm specifically referring to Ritalin and Adderall and modafinil, but more typically it's Adderall. Okay? People using cocaine and amphetamine for recreational purposes, that's a completely different beast, and it is indeed a beast, and it's something that I strongly discourage. However, I'm aware that up to 25% of college students, and perhaps as many as 35% of all individuals between the ages of 17 and 30, are taking Adderall on a regular or semiregular basis in order to work, in order to study and in order to function and focus in their daily life, even though they have not been diagnosed with ADHD.

Andrew Huberman:

There's a whole black market for this. They're getting it from people with prescriptions. I'm not here to pass judgment. I just want to emphasize how these drugs work, some of the things that they do to enhance cognition and focus that actually serve the brain well in certain individuals, and how they can be very detrimental in other individuals. I sort of blew right past it, but the fact that in upwards of 25% of young people are taking things like Adderall despite not having a clinical diagnosis of ADHD, well, that's a ridiculously high number. A few years ago, it was estimated that Adderall use and Ritalin use without diagnosis of ADHD was second in incident only to cannabis. But actually, now the consumption of Adderall without prescription is higher than the consumption of cannabis in that age group. So what that means is that there's a lot of stimulant use in that age group, and there are a lot of adults also using and abusing stimulants in order to gain focus.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, we could have a whole discussion about whether or not life is becoming more demanding, whether or not the need for focus is excessive, and that's why people are doing that. Frankly, it's an interesting discussion, but it's not one that would deliver us to any answers. Rather, I'd like to focus on the ways that people now, and people have always been self-medicating to increase focus, right? Caffeine, which I indulge some, I don't think to excess, has long been used as a stimulant to increase dopamine, increase norepinephrine, increase focus and energy. And in addition to that, it works through the so-called cyclic AMP phosphodiesterase pathway. Remember, anytime you see, you hear an A-S-E. that's an enzyme. Phosphodiesterase is involved in the conversion of things like cyclic AMP into energy for cells and so forth. Basically, coffee gives you energy, it makes you feel good, and it increases focus because of the circuits that it engages in the brain.

Andrew Huberman:

People have been taking caffeine, and continue to take caffeine, for ages. People also used to smoke cigarettes, nicotine, in order to gain focus. Nowadays, that's less common because of the concerns, quite valid concerns, about lung cancer from smoking. But there's a lot of vaping out there. There are a lot of people now consuming nicotine, which is the active substance in cigarettes, and in most nicotine vapes, that stimulates the brain to be more focused and more alert. So the idea of taking stimulants, of consuming things or smoking things in order to increase alertness is not a new idea. It's just that in ADHD, it's surprising that these things would work, right? I mean, if the problem is attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, what we're really talking about here, or children that are prescribed a drug that ought to be a stimulant, it ought to make them hyper hyperactive, and rather than doing that, it actually somehow serves to calm them a bit, or at least allow them to focus. Here's the reason. Children have a brain that's very plastic, meaning it can remodel itself and change in response to experience very, very quickly compared to adults. Taking stimulants as a child, if you are a child diagnosed with ADHD, allows that forebrain task-related network to come online, to be active at the appropriate times. And because those children are young, it allows those children to learn what focus is and to sort of follow or enter that tunnel of focus. Now, by taking a drug, it's creating focus artificially. It's not creating focus because they're super interested in something. It's chemically inducing a state of focus. And let's face it, a lot of childhood and school and becoming a functional adult is about learning how to focus even though you don't want to do something.

Andrew Huberman:

In fact, when I was in college, I had this little trick that may or may not work for some of you, which is if I couldn't focus on the material I was trying to learn, I would delude myself into thinking that it was the most interesting thing in the world. I would just kind of lie to myself and tell myself, "Okay, this ... ," I won't mention the subjects; "I absolutely love this." I would tell myself that I loved it, and I noticed that just that selective or deliberate engagement of that desire-to-know circuit, whatever that is in my brain, no doubt involves dopamine, allowed me to focus and remember the information. And somewhat surprisingly, or perhaps not surprisingly, I would often fall in love with the information. I'd find that that was my favorite class. It was what I wanted to learn the most.

Andrew Huberman:

So, that's one way you can do it artificially. But kids with ADHD, they can't do that, right? They're told to sit still and they end up getting up 11 times. They are told that they can't speak out in class or that they have to remain in their seats for 10 minutes, and they just, despite their best effort, they simply cannot do it. They're highly distractible. So what are we to make of this whole picture? That we need more dopamine, but these kids with ADHD, they're getting their dopamine by way of a drug, which is for all the world amphetamines, right? It's speed. That's really what it is. What are the long-term consequences? What are the short-term consequences? And what should we make of people taking these drugs without a clinical need? What are the consequences there?

Andrew Huberman:

Well, in order to get to some of those answers, I went to one of my colleagues. This is a colleague that I've actually known for a very long time. I was their teaching assistant when they were an undergraduate. They went on to get an MD, a medical degree, as well as a PhD and have become a pediatric neurologist that specializes in the treatment of epilepsy and ADHD in kids of all ages, from age three to 21. That's the age range, pretty broad age range, and has extensive knowledge in this. And what makes them particularly interesting, for sake of this discussion, is that they have a child, a young boy who's now showing signs of ADHD, and they are on the threshold of trying to decide whether or not they will prescribe Adderall or something similar.

Andrew Huberman:

So we had a discussion about this, and prior to learning that their child may have ADHD, I asked the following questions. First of all, I asked, What do you think about giving young kids amphetamine? And their answer was, on the face of it it seems crazy, but provided that the lowest possible dose is used and that that dosage is modulated as they grow older and developed those powers of attention, their observation was that they've seen more kids benefit than not benefit from that. Now, I'm certainly not saying what people should do. You obviously have to go to a doctor because as I always say, I'm not a doctor. I don't prescribe anything. I'm a professor, so I profess things. And here I'm professing that you talk to your doctor if you're considering giving Ritalin or Adderall or any type of stimulant to your child. Of course, what could be more important than the health of your child?

Andrew Huberman:

But it was a very interesting answer, because typically we hear, yes, medicate, or don't medicate. Rarely do we hear that the medication should be adjusted across the lifespan and in any particular kind of way. Now, the fact that this person, this now friend of mine and colleague of mine, has so much expertise in the way that the brain works and is considering putting their child on such medication, I said, "You know, why wouldn't you wait until your kid reaches puberty?"

Andrew Huberman:

I mean, we know that in boys and in girls there are increases in testosterone and estrogen during puberty that dramatically change the way that the body appears. But also that dramatically change the way that the brain functions. In particular, we know this: that puberty triggers the activation of so-called frontotemporal task-related executive functioning. That's just fancy science-speak for being able to focus, being able to direct your attention, being able to control your impulses. Look at a small child, or look at a puppy, and then look at an older child, or look at a dog. Very different levels, patterns of spontaneous behavior. Young children move around a lot, I don't want to say shifty because that makes it sound like they're up to something bad, which they might be, but they don't have to be up to something bad. They fidget a lot. So do puppies. Everything's a stimulus. As animals and humans get older, they learn how to control their behavior and sit still, listen and focus even if they don't want to.

Andrew Huberman:

So giving a drug that allows a child to access that stillness early on, it's thought will allow them to maintain that ability as time goes on. But I decided to push a little bit further. I said, "Well, why would you do it now as opposed to during puberty or after puberty?" And their answer was very specific, and I think very important. What they said was, "Look, neuroplasticity is greatest in childhood and tapers off after about age 25. But neuroplasticity from age three until age 12 or 13 is exceedingly high." And they're right.

Andrew Huberman:

When you sit back and you look at the literature on neuroplasticity, you'd say childhood plasticity and young adult plasticity is much greater than adult plasticity. But that early childhood plasticity is far and away the period in which you can reshape the brain at an accelerated rate. So this lines up really well with the clinical literature — not surprisingly, they're a clinician — that early treatment is key. If you have the opportunity to work with a quality physician and treat these things early, these drugs can allow these frontal circuits, these task-related circuits, to achieve their appropriate levels of functioning and for kids to learn how to focus in a variety of different contexts. Now, is that the only thing that they should be doing? Of course not.

Andrew Huberman:

So, the next question I asked was, "What should we make of all this diet-related stuff, right?" I've heard before that the so-called elimination diet, or ingesting no sugars, or no dairy, or no gluten, that all of these things have been purported to improve symptoms of ADHD. And people and parents with ADHD go to fanatic lengths to try and find the exact foods that are causing problems and the exact foods that the kids can eat in order to try and get their brain wired up right, and correctly, and to avoid lifelong ADHD. And their answer was really interesting. But before I tell you their answer, I want to tell you the studies and the data related to this question of whether or not food and the constellation of foods that one avoids and will eat has anything to do with our levels of attention. And in particular, whether or not that can be used as a leverage point to treat ADHD.

Andrew Huberman:

So you can imagine the challenges of exploring the role of diet and nutrition in any study, but especially in a study on ADHD. Why? Well, because as I mentioned before, children with ADHD, and it turns out adults with ADHD, tend to pursue sugary foods or any types of food that increase their levels of dopamine. They are naturally drawn to those foods, whether or not they realize it or not, presumably, as a way to try and treat their lack of focus and impulsivity. So in this study that I'm about to share with you, there was no drug treatment. It was just a study manipulating diet and involved a hundred children, 50 in the so-called elimination diet group, the special diet where certain foods were eliminated, and 50 in the so-called control group. However, being a well-designed randomized controlled trial, the study also included a crossover, meaning where the kids would serve as their own control or control group at a certain portion of the study. So they would be in one group where they eliminated certain foods, and then after a period of time in the study, they would swap to the other group. This is a powerful way to design a study for reasons that you can imagine, because you start to eliminate changes and effects due to individual differences.

Andrew Huberman:

In any case, a hundred children total, 50 in each group at any one period in time, and the effects that they observed were extremely dramatic. In the world of statistics and analysis of scientific data, we talk about P values, probability values. What's the likelihood that something could happen according to chance? And typically the cutoff would be something like p less than .05. That's less than .05 chance, essentially, of the effect being due to chance. However, in this study, every single one of the effects is p less than .0001. Very, very infinitesimally small probability that the effects observed could be due to chance. So what were these effects? These effects were enhanced ability to focus, less impulsivity, even less tendency to move when trying to sit still. So everything from mental focus to the ability to control their bodies improved when they were in the elimination diet group.

Andrew Huberman:

What was eliminated? Well, the elimination diet in this particular study was a so-called oligoantigenic diet. It was a diet in which each kid took a test to determine which foods they had antibodies for, meaning that they were mildly allergic to. Now, in this study, it was very important that the kids not be extremely allergic to any food because as I mentioned before, they actually served as a control at one point in the study where they were eating all sorts of foods, including foods that they had mild allergies to. So basically what this study said was that eliminating foods to which children have allergies can dramatically improve their symptoms of ADHD. And this study, not surprisingly because it was published in such a high-quality journal, Lancet, et cetera, large number of subjects, set the world on fire. People were extremely excited about these results, because here, in the absence of any drug treatment, there was a significant improvement in ADHD symptoms observed. And then came the criticisms.

Andrew Huberman:

So, many papers were published after this, specifically dealing with reanalysis of these data. And I want to be fair in saying that the data in the paper look good, but there are criticisms of the overall structural design in the study. I don't want to go into all the details exactly, because it gets really nuanced about some of the statistics in the way that one examines these types of data. But there was skepticism. And in science, skepticism is healthy, especially when making decisions about whether or not to treat or feed children one food or another or give them one drug or another. Now I want to return to the story of my friend who is a pediatric neurologist and treats ADHD and has a child who is on the precipice of perhaps starting to take drugs for the treatment of ADHD.

Andrew Huberman:

I asked the simple question, "Do you see an effect of diet?" Meaning when parents control the diet of their children, does it make a positive or negative or no difference in terms of the way that the kids respond to ADHD drugs like Ritalin and Adderall or whether or not it can help them avoid treating with those drugs entirely? And her response was very straightforward. She said, "Elimination of simple sugars has a dramatic and positive effect." She's observed that over and over and over again in many dozens if not hundreds of patients. Okay? Now that's not a peer-reviewed study, that's a statement that I'm conveying to you anecdotally, but it's a highly, highly informed one. I said, "What about these elimination diets?" She said, and I found other sources to support this, that these oligoantigenic diets are controversial. There are many people who really believe in identifying all the things that you're allergic to and making sure that you and, especially, your kids avoid those foods.

Andrew Huberman:

However, there's another camp that's starting to emerge in the peer-reviewed scientific literature showing that when kids are not exposed to certain foods, in particular nuts and things of that sort, they develop allergies to those foods and then when exposed to them later, they cause real problems. So there's a whole galaxy of discussion and controversy, and outright fighting, about allergies and kids, and whether or not the oligoantigenic diet is the appropriate one. However, out of the four neurologists and psychiatrists that I spoke to about ADHD in preparation for this, every single one said children with ADHD, as much as possible, should be encouraged to avoid high sugar and simple sugar foods of most kinds. And if they can find particular foods that exacerbate their symptoms, obviously eliminating those foods is beneficial, and the foods that exacerbate their symptoms change over time. So I don't like giving a complicated answer, but I also don't like giving an incomplete answer.

Andrew Huberman:

What this tells me is that children, and especially young children, who have ADHD should probably not eat much sugar, in particular simple sugars. In addition to that, exploring whether or not they have existing allergies to foods they already consume might be a good idea. At least that's what this paper, the Pelsser et al. Lancet paper, seems to speak to. And I should mention that that paper was published in 2011. Since then, there have been many dozens of studies exploring the same thing as well as meta-analysis of all those data. And it does appear that diet can have a highly significant role in eliminating or at least reducing the symptoms of ADHD, so much so that some of the children are able to not take medication at all or eventually wean themselves off medication as young adults and as adults. One interesting question is whether or not adults should modify their diet in order to increase their levels of focus if they're already having normal levels of focus but would like more, or would like to reduce existing adult ADHD.

Andrew Huberman:

That's an interesting and even more controversial topic. It brings us right into the realm of what are called omega-3 fatty acids. I've talked many times on this podcast about the known benefits of omega-3 fatty acids. In particular, getting one-gram, 1000 milligrams, or more, even as much as 2000 milligrams each day of the so-called EPA component of omega-3 fatty acids, known to have antidepressant effects, mood elevating effects, known to have important effects protecting the cardiovascular system.

Andrew Huberman:

I think it's now clear that the immune system also benefits, that omega-3 fatty acids that include a gram or more of EPAs are very beneficial. Typically, that's done through fish oil. Liquid fish oil is going to be the most cost-efficient. But there are capsule forms, for those of you that don't like fish oil, you can ingest this through other means. You can get it from certain algaes, or krill, et cetera. You have to make it compatible with your particular diet, whether or not you're vegan or vegetarian or omnivore, et cetera. Omega-3s have been shown to have all these positive health benefits. Do they have positive effects on focus and attention? And the answer is you can find studies that support that statement, and the effects are significant, but the effects are modest. You can also find studies that show no effect. However, much like with omega-3s and antidepressants, whereby ingestion of omega-3 fatty acids of a gram or more of EPA per day allows people with major depression to get away with taking lower doses of antidepressant medication. It does seem that ingestion of omega-3 fatty acids in, adults, that include EPAs of a thousand milligrams or more can allow adults with ADHD or mild attention deficit issues to function well on lower doses of medication, and in rare cases to eliminate medication entirely.

Andrew Huberman:

So what this says is, once again, that the omega-3 fatty acids are beneficial. Will they cure or eliminate ADHD? I think it's safe to say no. They are playing a supportive, or what we call a modulatory, role. Just like good sleep plays a supportive and modulatory role for essentially everything. Your immune system, your ability to think, your ability to regulate your emotion; it's modulating that process. This component of modulation is extremely important to highlight, and I think I want to spend a moment on it because this is especially important in the context of ADHD and all the information that's out there. There are biological processes that are mediated by particular compounds, like dopamine. So for instance, the ability to feel motivated, to attend, to focus is mediated by the circuits in the brain that release dopamine. However, attention is also modulated by how rested you are.

Andrew Huberman:

If you want to eliminate your ability to think well at all, just stay up for two nights and don't sleep at all, right? If you do that, you will have modulated the circuits in your brain that respond to various things, and you will be highly distractible. You will be highly emotional. You will feel like garbage. But that doesn't mean that sleep mediates focus and attention. It modulates it indirectly. Likewise, I think these omega-3 fatty acids, in particular the EPAs, which are so beneficial for mood and apparently also for attention, they don't directly mediate attention and mood. What they do is they modulate those circuits, they make dopamine more available. They make whatever dopamine is available more likely to bind to the various receptors that are present on neurons and so forth.

Andrew Huberman:

And I think this is very important because, likewise, diet and any discussion about nutrition has to include this framework of is the diet — the elimination diet, or whether or not it's some other diet or esoteric diet, ketogenic diet — is it modulating or mediating a process? And most likely in the context of ADHD, it's modulating that process. So if the ADHD is mild or if it's caught early enough, or if it's in conjunction with pharmacology with a prescription treatment, well then it might help guide the child or adult to a better place of being able to focus. But it's not going to be the switch that flips everything. Now, that does not mean that consuming the wrong foods, sugary foods or foods that you happen to be allergic to is a good idea. It will still be detrimental. So I hope that conceptual framework helps, because if you go online, if you're somebody with ADHD or not, you are going to be bombarded with the ADHD diet. The oligoantigenic diet. The "eliminate this supplement, that EPA," and I think it's very important to understand whether or not you're talking about something mediating a process or modulating a process.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, drugs like Ritalin, drugs like Adderall, they are tapping into the circuitries and the neurochemistries that mediate attention and focus. They are not the only alternatives, or the only choices rather, for treatment of these circuits and enhancement of the circuits for focus. I'm going to talk about other alternatives and some behavioral alternatives that are not very well-known, but are very, very effective, in a few minutes. But I really want to make this clear distinction between modulation and mediation because it's vital for anyone that's trying to modulate or mediate anything within their own brain. If any of you are interested in this oligoantigenic diet as it relates to ADHD, and you want to explore a more recent study besides that classic 2011 Lancet study that's rather controversial, there's a paper that was published in Frontiers in Psychiatry just last year, 2020. The title of the paper is "Oligoantigenic diet improves children's ADHD rating scale scores reliably in added video-rating." The added video-rating is just that they're using an additional measure of focus and attention. Again, that's Frontiers in Psychiatry, 2020. I'll put a link to it in the caption, and that's a more recent study for you to peruse.

Andrew Huberman:

So we've talked about the neural circuits of focus and the chemistry of focus, but we haven't talked yet about what would make us better at focusing and what focusing better really is. So let's take a step back and think about how we focus and how to get better at focus. And I'm going to share with you a tool for which there are terrific research data that will allow you in a single session to enhance your ability to focus, in theory, forever. What I'm about to read you is from an excellent book that I recommend if any of you are interested in neuroscience and things like meditation and default mode networks and things of that sort. The book is called "Altered Traits: Science Reveals How Meditation Changes Your Mind, Brain, and Body." And no, I'm not going to try and convince you to meditate.

Andrew Huberman:

I'm going to share with you a small passage in the book that relates some research data related to focus that are very important. If you want to meditate, that's your choice. That's a separate matter. This is a book by Daniel Goleman and Richard Davidson, and I should just mention that Goleman is a well-known author. He's written books on emotional intelligence and so forth. Richard Davidson is also a PhD. He's a professor of psychology and psychiatry, and that he's at University of Wisconsin-Madison. He's done terrific work on brain states and modulation of brain states and so forth.

Andrew Huberman:

... and modulation of brain states and so forth. What we're about to talk about is when attention works and when attention falters, and what we are specifically going to talk about are what are called attentional blinks. Not actual eye blinks, we're going to talk about that in a few minutes, but we're going to talk about attentional blinks. I'm paraphrasing here, because Goleman and Davidson wrote about this so beautifully, I'd rather paraphrase from them than try and just make up a new way to say it that is less interesting or less good, but I want to credit them. Attentional blinks are really easy to understand if you think about a Where's Waldo task. You know this task, Where's Waldo, where there are a bunch of people and objects and things in a picture, and somewhere in there is Waldo with the striped hat and the glasses, and kind of a skinny dude, and you have to find Waldo.

Andrew Huberman:

And so it's a visual search, and it's visual search for an object that has distinct features but is embedded in this ocean of other things that could easily be confused as Waldo. So you tend to look, look, look, look, look, look, look, look, look, and then you find Waldo. Kids can do this, they enjoy doing this. Adults may or may not enjoy it but they can do it too. They find Waldo. When you find Waldo, or when you search for a target in some other visual search task, at that moment your nervous system celebrates a little bit, and it celebrates through the release of neurochemicals that make you feel good. You found it, and you pause. Now the pause is interesting, because when you pause, what we know from many experiments is that in that moment of pause and mild celebration, however mild, you are not able to see another Waldo sitting right next to it.

Andrew Huberman:

So what this means is in attending to something, in searching and in identifying a visual target, your attention blinked. It shut off for a second. And there's a more formal and more laboratory-type way that we look at this. The more typical way to do this is to give someone a string of letters or a string of numbers, and beforehand you tell them, "Be on the lookout for the letters R and Z. Okay? You're just going to watch this string of numbers go by, and there will be a letter R in there, and there will be a letter Z in there, and try and spot them both." And what you find is when you present that string of numbers, and then they see the R, they see the R, they register it consciously, and they tend to miss the Z, just like in the Waldo-type example.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, of course the numbers are going by pretty quickly, but they can spot the R. They could also spot the Z if you told them beforehand, "Just spot the Z." And the numbers are moving through at the same rate in both conditions, so what that means is that in every case, you are capable of seeing the R or the Z. It's when you try and see both that seeing the first one prevents you from seeing the second one. It's what we call an attentional blink. We do this all the time, and people with ADHD tend to have many more attentional blinks than people that don't, and this is true for children and for adults. This is an important point, so important that I want to emphasize it twice in case you attentionally blinked. If you see something that you're looking for or you're very interested in something, you are definitely missing other information, in part because you're overfocusing on something.

Andrew Huberman:

And this leads to a very interesting hypothesis about what might go wrong in ADHD, where we've always thought that they cannot focus, and yet we know they can focus on things they care very much about. Well maybe, just maybe, they are experiencing more attentional blinks than people who do not have ADHD, and indeed there are data now to support the possibility that that's actually what's happening. And that should be exciting to anyone that has ADHD. It should also be exciting to anyone that cares about increasing their focus and their ability to attend.

Andrew Huberman:

What this is saying is that these circuits that underlie focus and our ability to attend and our ability to eliminate distraction, they aren't just failing to focus. That's just a semantic way of describing the outcome. They are overfocusing on certain things and thereby missing other things, and so our distractibility, or the distractibility of somebody with ADHD, could exist because they are overfocusing on certain elements, and they are there therefore missing other elements that they should be attending to.

Andrew Huberman:

So what they really need is this property that we call open monitoring. Now, open monitoring is something that's described in the book that I just referred to, and that typically is associated with people who have done a lot of meditation, so-called Vipassana meditation, or have spent a lot of time learning how to do what's called open gaze visual analysis and open gaze thinking, but there's a simpler version of this that allows us to bypass all that. First of all, your visual system has two modes of processing. It can be highly focused, a soda-straw view, so looking for the R in this string of numbers in the example that I just gave. Or if you're very excited about something, you're in that soda-straw view of the world, and you're missing other things. Okay? That's high levels of attention. However, there's also a property of your visual system that allows you to dilate your gaze, to be in so-called panoramic vision.

Andrew Huberman:

Panoramic vision is something you can do right now, no matter where you are, and I can do it right now. You won't know that I'm doing it, but even though I'm still looking directly at you, I'm consciously dilating my gaze so that I can see the ceiling, the floor, and the walls all around me. That panoramic vision is actually mediated by a separate stream or set of neural circuits going from the eye into the brain, and it's a stream or set of circuits that isn't just wide angle view. It also is better at processing things in time. Its frame rate is higher. So you've seen slow motion video and you've seen standard video. Slow motion video gives you that slow motion look because it's a higher frame rate, you're thin slicing time. Okay? You can use panoramic vision to access the state that we call open monitoring.

Andrew Huberman:

When people do that, they are able to attend to and recognize multiple targets within this string of numbers. They can see the R, and they can see the Z and they can see additional things. So this is something that can be trained up and people can practice, whether or not they have ADHD or not. What it involves is learning how to dilate your gaze consciously. That's actually quite easy for most people. Whether or not you wear corrective lenses or contacts or not, you can consciously go into open gaze and then you can contract your field of view as well.

Andrew Huberman:

There have also been studies done where people were taught to think in a particular way for a very short period of time, and that forever changed their ability to limit or reduce the number of these attentional blanks. There are now published accounts in the literature of a simple practice done for about 15 minutes where subjects were asked to just sit quietly, eyes closed, and do what is sort of akin to meditation, but to not direct their mind into any particular state or place, but simply to think about their breathing and to focus on their so-called interoception, focus on how their body feels, if their mind drifted to bring it back — okay, so it's basically meditation — for about 15 minutes.

Andrew Huberman:

That might not seem like a significant or unusual practice, or that it would have any impact at all, but remarkably, just doing that once for 17 minutes significantly reduced the number of attentional blinks that people would carry out, in other words their focus got better, in a near permanent way without any additional training. There's something about that practice of reducing the amount of visual information coming in and learning to pay attention to one's internal state, what we call interoception, that allowed them an awareness such that when they needed to look for visual targets, when they needed to focus on multiple things in sequence, they didn't experience the same number of attentional blinks. And I should mention, not incidentally, as people age and their working memory gets worse and their ability to focus gets worse, the number of attentional blinks that they carry out goes up.

Andrew Huberman:

And there are now studies exploring whether or not this simple meditation-like practice of 15 to 20 minutes or so, of sitting and just quietly resting and paying attention to one's breathing and internal state, can also offset some of that age-related, what is called, cognitive decline. So what these data tell mean is that regardless of whether or not you're a child or you're an adult, whether or not you have ADHD or not, whether or not you're experiencing age-related cognitive decline or you would simply like to avoid age-related cognitive decline, a simple practice of taking 17 minutes, sitting and paying attention to your internal state, just interocepting, registering your breathing, registering the contact of your skin with whatever surface you're on, can forever rewire your brain to be able to attend better, and possibly even offset some of that age-related attentional drift.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, I don't expect anyone to start meditating regularly. I don't expect anyone to do anything they don't want to do, but I think most of us could handle one meditation session of 17 minutes or so. And so if ever there was a tool that stood to rewire our attentional circuitry in a powerful way, this seems to be it. And in addition, the ability to engage in panoramic vision, to dilate our gaze, this so-called open monitoring that allows the brain to function in a way that it can detect more information faster, that's a powerful tool as well, and the beauty of that tool is that it works the first time, and it works every time. Now, how exactly it works is a little bit unclear. Is it, for instance, orchestrating this synchrony or asynchrony between the default mode network and the task-related networks? We don't know.

Andrew Huberman:

Those studies have not yet been carried out. Nonetheless, the effects are significant, they are long lasting, and they appear to exist after just one session of this quiet 17-minute interoception, which to me makes it seem like a very worthwhile thing to do for everybody. So we just talked about attentional blinks, which are essentially blinks of thinking. It's your mind shutting off for a moment and missing information. Now let's talk about actual blinks, the sort that you do with your eyelids. Now this might come across as somewhat obvious, but you can do fast, what are called spontaneous blinks, and they're always coordinated between the two eyes, or you can do long blinks, like when you go to sleep at night you do one very long blink. And I'm not being facetious. When you go to sleep at night, you are shutting your eyelids, and you are limiting the amount of information coming in, and your perception of time starts to drift as you go into sleep.

Andrew Huberman:

Your perception of time changes from very fast at one moment to very slow, meaning the frame rate at which you are analyzing information, dreaming, et cetera, is variable when you are in sleep. Sometimes it's very fast, meaning you experience things in slow motion, sometimes it's very fast. In waking too, your experience of time can sometimes be very fast, sometimes be very slow. Typically, the more alert you are, the higher the frame rate, you're thin slicing your experience. You've probably had this happen. If you're ever very stressed and you're waiting for something or somebody, it seems like it takes forever, because your frame rate is higher, you're analyzing time more finely. Conversely, if you are very relaxed or even sleepy, you wake up and you have to think about all the things you have to do, it will seem like the world is going by very, very fast and that you are moving very slow.

Andrew Huberman:

Time is going at the same rate, but your perception of time is what's changed. Believe it or not, your perception of time is also changed on a rapid basis, moment-to-moment basis, by how often you blink. This is a well-established literature in the world of neuroscience, that unlike the literature and claims about blinking and sociopathy which have no basis, the science of blinking as it relates to time perception has some very good data to support it. I want to just emphasize one study in particular, which is quite appropriately titled, "Time dilates after spontaneous blinking." This is a paper that was published in current biology. The first author is Terhune, T-E-R-H-U-N-E. It's a wonderful paper. They examine the relationship between fluctuations in timing and blinking. And to make a long story short, what they found is that right after blinks, we reset our perception of time. Okay?

Andrew Huberman:

So blinks in that sense are a little bit like the curtain coming down on a scene between scenes in a play, or takes in a movie. You know, when they clap the clap thing, they start it, "Take!" What do they say? "Action." And then at the end, they do the thing and they click it down and they say, "It's a take." That's one take. When you blink, it's a take. Okay? Now what's interesting, and will immediately make sense to you as to why this is important, is that the rate of blinking is controlled by dopamine. So what this means is that dopamine is controlling attention, blinks relate to attention and focus, and therefore the dopamine and blinking system is one way that you constantly modulate and update your perception of time, and fortunately, it's also one that you can control. So the basic takeaway of this study was that blinking controls time perception, but also that levels of dopamine can alter your sense of time, and, stay with me here, and that blinking and dopamine are inextricably linked. They are working together to control your attention.

Andrew Huberman:

When dopamine levels go up, people tend to overestimate how long something lasted. Why? Because they are processing time more finely. It's slow motion mode. When dopamine levels are lower, they tend to underestimate time intervals. Let's remember back to the very beginning of the episode. What's going on in people with ADHD? They are not good at managing their time. They tend to run late, or they're disorganized. They are not just disorganized in space, meaning in the physical space around them, they're disorganized in time. Their dopamine is low, we know that as well. And so they are underestimating time intervals, and so it makes perfect sense that they would be late. It makes perfect sense that they would lose track of time or the ability to focus.

Andrew Huberman:

This is really exciting, because what it means is that children with ADHD, adults with ADHD, or people with normal levels of focus that want to improve their ability to focus, can do so through a training that involves learning how often to blink and when, and how to keep their visual focus on a given target. And it turns out this study has actually been done. There's a study, again I'll link to this study, entitled "Improvement of attention in elementary school students through fixation focus training activity." And I won't go through all the details, but what they found was a short period of focusing on a visual target allowed these school children to greatly enhance their ability to focus on other types of information, and a significant component of the effect was due to the way that they were controlling the shutters on their eyes, their eyelids, and controlling their blinks.

Andrew Huberman:

So what they did in this study is they had these kids focus their visual attention on some object that was relatively close, like their hand, for a minute or so, which actually takes some effort if you try and do that. They were allowed to blink. However, it's known from other work that if people can consciously override the desire to blink, at least to the point where they feel like they have to or else their eyes will dry out, that actually can increase attention even further. And they had conditions where they would look at a point further across the room, and even further across the room.

Andrew Huberman:

It only took a few minutes each day to do this, 30 seconds in one condition, or maybe a minute, and then at another station of looking a little bit further out and a little bit further out. However, there was an important feature of this study that is definitely worth mentioning, which is before they did this visual focus task or training, they did a series of physical movements with the kids so that the kids could sort of eliminate or move out some of their desire to move and would thereby enhance their ability to sit still. Now it's long been known that kids need a recess, they need time to run around and play and roll around and do whatever it is that they do in order to be able to sit still at all.

Andrew Huberman:

Adults probably need this too, frankly, but kids need it more, because the circuits in the brain that control reflexive movements, and as we say kind of rhythmic, undulating behavior and things like that, that's an active suppression, and kids have less of that circuitry built up until they hit about age 15 or 16. So they had the kids move around a bit and then do this focus training. That brings me to another treatment that's actively used nowadays in schools for kids with ADHD, but also is starting to be used by many kids, and by parents in order to keep their kids focusing and not going crazy in the car or not acting out in general, and that's the prevalence of these so-called fidgeter toys, or things that kids can do actively and repetitively in order to move out some of their underlying reverberatory activity in their nervous system.

Andrew Huberman:

So what you will find is that some kids with ADHD are now given a rubber band on their desk, literally a rubber band that's attached to their desk, and they're able to pull on it, even snap it against the desk. If I had done that when I was a kid, I think my teachers would have thrown me out of class, but I think it's great that they're allowing them to do this now, as a way of moving some of their physical energy out, or engage their physical energy rather, as opposed to trying to sit statue still all the time and attend. And it turns out that does enhance these children's ability to focus mentally when they have some physical activity to attend to, and it turns out it also can work for adults. I'll share with you a related anecdote, because it illustrates the underlying mechanism. I've had the great privilege of being able to do a number of surgeries, brain surgeries, during my career.

Andrew Huberman:

So one thing you find when you do brain surgeries is that the brain's pretty small, regardless of the species that you're working on, and you're in there and you're trying to do something very specific, and the more you try and hold your hands really steady, the more they want to shake. All right? So it's not natural for any of our limbs to sit perfectly still. Depending on how much coffee you've had, how well-rested you are, and your sort of baseline level of autonomic arousal, some of you may find that you can hold out your hand absolutely rock solid, others will shake a little bit more. Doesn't mean you're nervous if you're shaking, doesn't mean you're calm if you're still. What it relates to is the amount of what we call premotor activity, the number of commands to move that are being sent through the system, and that's what I mean by reverberatory activity.

Andrew Huberman:

And it does seem that kids with ADHD and adults with ADHD have a lot of reverberatory activity in their nervous system, and so that's that constant desire to move, it's hard for them to sit still, and therefore it's hard for them to attend, to harness their attention. When you do a surgery and you find that your hands are shaking, what you learn from your mentors, which I did, and what works extremely well, whether or not you're doing a surgery or not, is that you simply tap your foot or you bounce your knee a little bit, which you might think would make your hand shake even more, but provided that it's subtle, what it does is it is actually shuttles some of the activity from those premotor circuits to elsewhere in the body, and then you're able to sit much more still with your hand.

Andrew Huberman:

You're able to perform the surgery with much more precision. You are able to write with much better handwriting. And for those of you who engage in public speaking, if you're ever too nervous, that's why pacing while you public speak helps if you're nervous. That's why bouncing your knee behind the podium works as well. That's why nodding your head and gesticulating can help. It's not a matter of "moving energy out of the body," that doesn't actually happen. What it is you're engaging those premotor circuits that are sending through commands. It's like trying to stuff a bunch of stuff through a funnel, and it creates this tension, so you're giving it an outlet for the neural circuitry to be able to move something so that you can keep other components of your body and your mental attention engaged and locked onto something, what we call focus.

Andrew Huberman:

One thing related to this whole business of blinking and focus, and training yourself to focus and not blinking, et cetera, is that most all of the drugs, Ritalin, Adderall, and recreational drugs that increase dopamine, even coffee and tea and other forms of caffeine, they tend to make us blink less. And when we get tired, we tend to blink more. Now, this is sort of a duh, right? But being wide-eyed with excitement or fear, or with your eyes barely being able to keep them open, now it should make perfect sense that these shutters on the front of your eyes, they aren't just there for winking, and they aren't just there for cosmetic purposes.

Andrew Huberman:

They are there to regulate the amount of information going into your nervous system, and they're there to regulate how long you are bringing information into your nervous system, and in what bins. How widely or finely you are binning time is set by how often you blink, and how widely or specifically you are grabbing attention from the visual world is set by whether or not you're viewing things very specifically, like a crosshair or through a soda-straw view, like this, or whether or not you are in this panoramic sort of whole environment mode, this kind of fish-eye lens or wide-angle lens mode.

Andrew Huberman:

And in fairness to the pharmacology and the circuitry, while dopamine and heightened levels of alertness and excitement tend to make us blink less and attend more, there's actually a study that's looked at the other neurochemical systems and drugs and how those relate to blinking, and so this will all be obvious by the title of the paper I'm about to share with you. This is a paper entitled, "Decreased spontaneous eye blink rates in chronic cannabis users: Evidence for striatal cannabinoid-dopamine interactions."

Andrew Huberman:

Okay, I'm not going to go into all the details here, but one thing that is somewhat surprising is that many people with ADHD use or abuse cannabis. You might think, "Well, why would they do that, because I thought that an increase in dopamine is actually what's going to lead to heightened levels of attention, and that's what these people and children crave." Well, it turns out that cannabis also increases dopamine transmission in the brain, but because of the other chemicals it increases, namely serotonin and some components of the cannabinoid and opioid system, it creates that kind of alert but mellow feel.

Andrew Huberman:

And again, here I'm not a proponent of this. I personally am not a THC or cannabis user, it's just not my thing. And obviously, it's illegal some places, and so you have to determine that for yourself. It does have medical purposes and in some places it is legal. But THC increases dopamine and increases neurochemicals that can also create a state of calm, so it's that sort of middle ground. And this paper has a beautiful demonstration whereby not just while people are using cannabis, but depending on how long they've been using cannabis across their lifespan, the rates of eye blinking change. So if you look at the number of years that people have been using cannabis on a regular basis, either weekly or up to daily, what you find is that, for people that have not been using cannabis at all or have only been using it for about two years, their rates of eye blinks are much higher than people who have been using it chronically for 10 years.

Andrew Huberman:

In other words, people who have been using cannabis for 10 years don't blink very often at all. Now, cannabis has well-known effects in depleting memory, but it does seem to engage the focus and blinking system in a way that increases focus. So basically what I'm saying is marijuana seems to increase people's focus, but then they can't remember what they were focusing on. Something I'd like to discuss just briefly is the so-called interoceptive awareness that's present in people with ADHD, both children and adults. Interoceptive awareness is one's sense of one's own internal state, heartbeat, breathing, contact of skin with a given surface, et cetera. For a long time there was this hypothesis, this idea that people with ADHD were just not in touch with how they felt, that somehow they weren't registering all the stuff that was going on inside them, changes in heart rate and so forth, and so they were behaving in a way that was dysregulated, or appeared dysregulated, and that if they could just learn to attend to their internal state better, that somehow they would function better in the world.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, before we described a process, literally a 17-minute interceptive exercise, that does seem to lead to improvements in one's ability to focus for a longer period of time. However, it's very unlikely that that was due to increasing interoceptive awareness per se. It probably wasn't because people gain a much heightened or improved ability to understand what's going on internally. In fact, you can imagine how that might actually prevent one's ability to pay attention to things in the outside world. So while there is benefit to just sitting there and being in stillness, as they say, or focusing on one's breathing and internal state for sake of then accessing information in the external world, a really nice study called, "Interoceptive awareness in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder," explored whether or not interceptive awareness was different in people with ADHD, or did not have ADHD.

Andrew Huberman:

And the findings were essentially that there's no difference, that people with ADHD, children and adults, they are aware of what's going on inside them just as much as anyone else is. And the typical measure of interoceptive awareness is one's ability to count their own heartbeats. This is actually challenging for some individuals and very easy for other individuals, regardless of their attentional capacity. Some people just can really feel their heartbeat without taking their pulse. Other people cannot. And these studies are pretty straightforward to do. You ask people to sit there and to count their heartbeats, and then you are monitoring their heartbeats, and you get to gauge how accurate they are.

Andrew Huberman:

So it's important to understand that people with ADHD are in touch with how they feel. It's really a question of whether or not they can take the demands that are placed upon them and enter a cognitive state, a mental state, that allows them to access the information they need to access. In other words, whether or not they can focus. But it is absolutely wrong to think that the child that's getting up 11 times during a short six-minute interaction at the table, or whether or not a child who somehow has to venture off every moment, or a coworker of yours who's an adult who's constantly fidgeting or moving things around, that somehow they are unaware, that they are oblivious.

Andrew Huberman:

They're not oblivious to how they feel. Chances are they are very challenged in the situations that they're in, and they're doing everything they can to try and regulate their attention. So I think it's an important study to highlight because it really underscores the fact that something else is going on, and that something else has everything to do with this ability to coordinate these task-directed networks and to coordinate that in the proper way with that default mode network.

Andrew Huberman:

And that is a process, as you now know, that's regulated exquisitely by certain neurochemicals, and in particular the neurochemicals dopamine, norepinephrine and serotonin. And a fourth one I'd like to throw into the mix which is acetylcholine, which is very vital for cognitive focus. So now I want to switch back to talking about some of the drugs that are typically used to access those systems, prescription drugs, and I want to talk about some of the new and emerging nonprescription approaches to increasing the levels of dopamine, acetylcholine and serotonin in the brain using various supplement-type compounds, because several of them are showing really remarkable efficacy in excellent peer-reviewed studies.

Andrew Huberman:

So before moving to some of the newer atypical compounds and things sold over the counter, I'd like to just briefly return to the classic drugs that are used to treat ADHD. These are the ones I mentioned earlier, methylphenidate, also called Ritalin; modafinil; armodafinil is another one and Adderall. Again, all of these work by increasing levels of dopamine and norepinephrine. Typically, they are taken orally in pill form, or sometimes in capsule form. The dosages that are appropriate vary according to severity of the condition, for a given person, and the age of the person. This is a complicated landscape for each individual. They have to figure out the pharmacology that's best for them. Some individuals are even layering long or timed release Ritalin with Adderall in smaller doses. It can get quite complex, or it can be quite straightforward.

Andrew Huberman:

If you are really interested in these drugs and how they work, and you'd like to get a glance at a table of all the results from all the studies, of which there are now hundreds, there's an excellent review about these drugs and their use, and their comparison to similarly structured drugs, in particular MDMA and cocaine and amphetamine, meaning street amphetamine, to really illustrate the similarities of action and some of the problems associated with long-term use. I don't expect you to read this article in full. I'm here so that you don't have to go read these articles, but in case you want a ton of information, the paper is Esposito et al, Frontiers in Biosciences. It's an excellent, excellent review of the entire literature. It is quite long, I can put a link to that study in our caption, and it essentially describes all the studies that have been done, peer reviewed and published, and it refers to these drugs in an interesting way.

Andrew Huberman:

It doesn't just refer to these drugs as for treatment of ADHD. It actually refers to them using language that ordinarily I'm not very fond of but I'll agree to here, which is so-called smart drugs or nootropics. It also covers caffeine, which again, as I mentioned earlier, increases dopamine, norepinephrine and, to some extent, serotonin. But what I like about this review so much is that in putting these drugs of abuse, methamphetamine and cocaine, right alongside these drugs like Ritalin and Adderall, and also caffeine, we start to realize that the distinction between drugs of abuse and the distinction between drugs of treatment is actually a very fine and sometimes even a blurry line.

Andrew Huberman:

And in thinking about whether or not one wants to use these prescription — I want to emphasize prescription, not drugs of abuse, but prescription drugs — for treatment of one's own attentional capacity, I think it is important to understand the extent to which they all carry more or less the same side effects. The one exception being caffeine. Caffeine's side effects can be anxiety if you ingest too much of it, insomnia if you drink it too late in the day, but typically it will not cause the major side effects of the other drugs, such as high propensity for addiction and abuse. Amphetamines of any kind, as well as cocaine, can cause sexual side effects because they're vasoconstrictors, so men have trouble achieving erection. There can often be the intense desire or libido for sex but an inability to actually perform, so that's an issue with any kind of stimulant.

Andrew Huberman:

So these drugs are not without their consequences. In addition, and here I'd lump caffeine back into the mix, in addition they almost all carry cardiac effects, right? They increase heart rate, but they also have effects on constriction of blood vessels and arteries and veins and so forth in ways that can create cardiovascular problems. Now, caffeine is a bit of a complicated one. I talked about this on a podcast long ago, but I'll just remind you that it turns out that if you are caffeine adapted, in other words if you are used to drinking caffeine, then the ingestion of caffeine most often will cause vasodilation, it will actually allow more blood flow through. However, if you are not caffeine adapted, it will cause vasoconstriction due to an increased stress response. So if you're familiar with caffeine, caffeine can actually have a little bit more of a relaxation response, although if you drink enough of it will make you amped up. These other drugs almost always lead to vasoconstriction, increased-

Andrew Huberman:

Drugs almost always lead to vasoconstriction, increased heart rate, dilation of the pupils, less blinking, heightened levels of attention, which looks very much like stress. And at its extremes looks very much like the effects of street drugs like cocaine and amphetamine. Because of the large amounts of dopamine that released in the brain, people tend to crave that state over and over, and yet with each subsequent use are able to get less and less of that euphoric feeling, or that really, really focused feeling. So one thing that's being explored quite extensively now in the treatment of ADHD are drug schedules. Whether or not people should take Adderall every day or every other day. Whether or not they should take it only every once in a while. Whether or not young children can take it just a few times and engage in behavioral training of the sort that I talked about before, where they're doing, maybe it's a 17-minute meditation-type exercise, but more likely it would be the movement followed by the visual focusing, because that's only done for 20 or 30 or 60 seconds.

Andrew Huberman:

Why would you do that? Well, in a chemically enhanced state, your brain is more plastic. The circuits are able to modify and learn better. That's the optimal time to engage in focus in a very deliberate way. So just taking a drug and expecting focus to just work at any point and being able to turn focus on and off at will, that's an unrealistic expectation. Right? More likely the best use of things like Adderall, modafinil, armodafinil, and Ritalin is going to be to combine those treatments with behavioral exercises that actively engage the very circuits that you're trying to train up and enhance. And then perhaps, I want to highlight perhaps, tapering off those drugs so that then one can use those circuits without any need for chemical intervention. So despite any controversy that might be out there, I think it's fair to say that the consumption of omega-3 fatty acids can positively modulate the systems for attention and focus.

Andrew Huberman:

So then the question becomes how much EPA? How much DHA? Does that differ for what's helpful for depression, et cetera? And actually it does differ. In reviewing the studies for this, it appears that a threshold level of 300 milligrams of DHA turns out to be an important inflection point. So typically, fish oils or other sources of omega-3s will have DHA and EPA. And typically it's the EPA that's harder to get at sufficient levels. Meaning you have to take quite a lot of fish oil in order to get above that 1000 milligram or 2000 milligram threshold to improve mood and other functions. But for sake of attention, there are 10 studies that have explored this in detail. And while the EPA component is important, the most convincing studies point to the fact that getting above 300 milligrams per day of DHA is really where you start to see the attentional effects.

Andrew Huberman:

Now, fortunately, if you're getting sufficient EPA, for sake of mood and other biological functions, almost without question, you're getting 300 milligrams or more of DHA. So that usually checks that box just fine. What's interesting is that there's another compound, phosphatidylserine, that has been explored for its capacity to improve the symptoms of ADHD. Again, I don't think this is in a direct way, but rather in a modulatory way. But it appears that phosphatidylserine, taken for two months, for 200 milligrams per day,` was able to reduce the symptoms of ADHD in children. It has not been looked at in adults yet. At least as far as I know, but that this effect was greatly enhanced by the consumption of omega-3 fatty acids. So now we're starting to see synergistic effects of omega-3 fatty acids and phosphatidylserine. Again, that was 200 milligrams per day.

Andrew Huberman:

This is something that sold over the counter in capsule form, at least in the U.S. There were two studies. Both were double-blind studies, carried out for anywhere from one to six months on both boys and girls. And it really was boys and girls, not men and women. This was kids age one to six or seven to twelve. And it was a fairly large number of subjects. So 147 subjects in one case and 36 in the other. The takeaway is that getting sufficient levels of EPAs, in particularly this 300 milligram threshold of DHA. Plus, if you are interested in it and it's right for you, 200 milligrams of phosphatidylserine, can be an important augment for improving the symptoms of ADHD. You'll also find literature out there and many claims about so-called ginkgo biloba, which has been shown to have minor effects in improving the symptoms of ADHD.

Andrew Huberman:

Not nearly as effective as Ritalin and Adderall. Gingko biloba is not appropriate for many people. I am one such person. I don't have ADHD, but when I've taken gingko, even at very low doses, I get absolutely splitting headaches. Some people do not experience those headaches, but it's known to have very potent vasoconstrictive and vasodilating properties that vary. Depending on when you took the compound. So for those of you that are exploring gingko biloba, and you will see a lot of claims about gingko biloba for attention and ADHD, definitely take the vasodilation, vasoconstriction headache issue into consideration.

Andrew Huberman:

So I'd like to talk about the drug modafinil, and the closely related drug armodafinil. That's A-R-modafinil. Because modafinil and armodafinil are gaining popularity out there both for treatment of ADHD and narcolepsy, but also for communities of people that are trying to stay awake long periods of time.

Andrew Huberman:

So it's actively used in the military, by first responders, it's gaining popularity on college campuses and people are using it more and more as an alternative to Adderall and Ritalin and excessive amounts of coffee. It does increase focus, and to a dramatic extent. Modafinil typically was very expensive. I don't know if it's still this expensive, but when one has a prescription for it, it could still cost as much as eight or nine hundred dollars, or even a thousand dollars a month. Armodafinil is a far less expensive version that's chemically slightly different than Modafinil. Regardless of price, people are taking modafinil and armodafinil. I want to emphasize that unlike Ritalin and Adderall, modafinil and armodafinil are weak dopamine reuptake inhibitors, and that's how they lead to increases in dopamine. So, whereas Ritalin and Adderall, amphetamine and cocaine lead to big increases in dopamine, also through reuptake mechanisms, and so forth. Modafinil is a weaker dopamine reuptake stimulator.

Andrew Huberman:

And so what that means is that it leaves more dopamine around to be active at the synapse, the gaps between neurons. However, it also activates other systems. It acts on the orexin system, which is actually a peptide that we talked about in the episode on hunger. Because it regulates hunger and appetite. And it regulates sleepiness and feelings of sleepiness. In fact the orexin, also called hypocretin system, the orexin hypocretin system, is what's disrupted in narcolepsy. That was the important discovery of my colleagues Emmanuel Mignot and Seiji Nishino at Stanford some years ago. They identified the biological basis of narcolepsy, and it's a disruption in this orexin hypocretin system. And modafinil is one of the primary treatments for narcolepsy. It also has these other effects on the dopamine system and on the norepinephrine system. Even though it doesn't lead to quite as intense levels of dopamine and arousal and focus, it does have the property of raising levels of attention and focus, and that's why people are using it.

Andrew Huberman:

So it's a somewhat milder form of Adderall. Armodafinil, for some people, works as well as modafinil — and as I mentioned before, it's much lower cost — for other people it doesn't. I have an experience, meaning I do have an experience that I'll share with you with armodafinil. A few years ago, I was suffering from jet lag really terribly, and I was traveling overseas. I went to a meeting to give a talk. I took half of the prescribed dose of armodafinil. It was prescribed to me. I took that half dose and I gave my lecture, and then I stayed around to answer questions. And then four hours later, a friend of mine came up to me and said, "You've been talking for four and a half hours, and there are only a few people still here." Luckily, there were still a few people, it would be a lot weirder if the room was completely empty since it wasn't being recorded.

Andrew Huberman:

So I have firsthand knowledge of the sorts of cognitive effects that it can create. I personally would not want to be in that state for sake of studying or learning or for doing this podcast, for instance. And I can honestly say, that today all I've ingested is some coffee and some yerba maté tea and some water. I'm not on any of the compounds that I've described during the course of today's episode.

Andrew Huberman:

You might ask why I took half the recommended dose of armodafinil, and the reason is that I'm somebody who's fairly hypersensitive to medication of any kind. What you find if you look in the literature is that about 5% of people are hyper, hypersensitive to medication. They require far lower doses of any medication than other people in order to experience the same effects. I'm somebody that I think is sort of modest hyper, if that's sort of an oxymoronic statement, but modest hypersensitivity to medication.

Andrew Huberman:

So I've almost always been able to get by on taking less of whatever was prescribed for me and feel just fine, or in this case to feel like it was still too much. It turned out that the right dose of armodafinil for me was zero milligrams.

Andrew Huberman:

Now you may notice that I haven't talked much about acetylcholine. Acetylcholine is a neurotransmitter that at the neuron-to-muscle connections, the so-called neuromuscular junctions, is involved in generating muscular contractions of all kinds for all movements. Acetylcholine is also released from two sites in the brain. So a little bit of nomenclature here. Again, feel free to ignore the nomenclature, but there is a collection of neurons in your brain stem that send projections forward, kind of like a sprinkler system that's very diffuse, to release acetylcholine. And those neurons reside in an area or a structure that's called the pedunculopontine nucleus, the PPN.

Andrew Huberman:

And then there's a separate collection of neurons in the basal forebrain called, unimaginatively, nucleus basalis, the nucleus at the base. And they also hose the brain with acetylcholine, but in a much more specific way. So one is sort of like a sprinkler system, and the other one is more like a fire hose to a particular location. And those two sources of acetylcholine collaborate to activate particular locations in the brain and really bring about a tremendous degree of focus to whatever is happening at those particular synapses. So it could be a focus on visual information or auditory information. If you're listening closely to what I'm saying right now, you just heard "closely" step out from the rest of my sentence. No doubt there was acetylcholine released at the sites in your brain where the neurons that represent your recognition of the word "closely" occurred.

Andrew Huberman:

Okay, so now you have an example and you have an understanding and hopefully a picture in your mind of how all this is working. Not surprisingly then, drugs that increase cholinergic or acetylcholine transmission will increase focus and cognition. One such compound is so called Alpha-GPC. Which is a form of choline and increases acetylcholine transmission. Dosages as high as 1200 milligrams per day, which is a very high dosage, spread out, typically it's 300 or 400 milligrams, spread out throughout the day, have been shown to offset some of the effects of age-related cognitive decline, improve cognitive functioning in people that don't have age-related cognitive decline. That's a very high dose. Typically, when people are using Alpha-GPC to study or to enhance learning of any kind, they will take somewhere between 300 and 600 milligrams. That's more typical. Again, you have to check with your doctor. You have to decide if the safety margins are appropriate for you. Obviously, you'd want to check that out. But Alpha-GPC is effective in creating more focus by way of this cholinergic system.

Andrew Huberman:

It stimulates acetylcholine release from both of those locations. The PPN in the back of the brain and nucleus basalis in the front of the brain. There are two other over-the-counter compounds that are in active use out there for treatment of ADHD and in use for simply trying to improve focus. And the first one is L-tyrosine, it's an amino acid that acts as a precursor to the neuromodulator dopamine. And now, knowing everything you know about dopamine attention and the circuits involved, it should come as no surprise as to why people are exploring the use of L-tyrosine for that purpose. L-tyrosine does lead to increases in dopamine. They are fairly long-lived, and L-tyrosine can improve one's ability to focus. However, the dosaging can be very tricky to dial in. Sometimes it makes people feel too euphoric, or too jittery, or too alert, that they are then unable to focus well.

Andrew Huberman:

So the dosage ranges are huge. You see evidence for a hundred milligrams all the way up to 1200 milligrams. It's something that really should be approached with caution. Especially for people that have any kind of underlying psychiatric or mood disorder, because dysregulation of the dopamine system is central to many of the mood disorders, such as depression, but also especially mania, mania bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, things of that sort. So it's something that really should be approached with caution. Nonetheless, in exploring what's out there, and even some studies online that were done, either animal studies or human studies, it's clear that L-tyrosine is being explored for that purpose. As is PEA and phenylethylamine, which is essentially PEA, but some related compounds. So there's a whole class of dopaminergic or dopamine-stimulating supplements that people are using to try and get their dopamine levels up.

Andrew Huberman:

And again, it's a kind of a fine line between too little, enough and too much. If you want to get the literature on those two compounds there, I will refer to this great website, examine.com. Just as it sounds, and you can put in L-tyrosine or PEA and you can get the details on that. But I highly recommend also going to their section on ADHD to see how those particular compounds relate specifically to ADHD and cognitive focus. And last but not least, in terms of these different compounds. I do want to mention the racetams. These are somewhat esoteric and probably most of you haven't heard about them, but some of you probably know a lot about them, and they are becoming more popular. They go by names like Noopept, and things of that sort. The racetams are illegal in certain countries. They are gray market in other countries, and they're sold over the counter in this country, in the U.S.

Andrew Huberman:

So they have different margins for safety. You definitely need to consult your doctor, especially if you have ADHD. But Noopept has been shown, when taken at 10 milligrams twice daily, can be more effective than some of the other racetams. What is Noopept? Noopept taps into the cholinergic system, the acetylcholine system, in ways very similar to Alpha-GPC, but seems to have a slightly higher affinity for some of the receptors involved and can lead to those heightened states of cognitive capacity. And there are these studies, one in particular, comparative studies of Noopept and piracetam and the treatment of patients with mild cognitive disorders and brain disease of vascular and traumatic origin. That's a mouthful. What this study basically points to is the fact that people who are experiencing some degree of inability to focus, due to prior concussion or some vascular event, a stroke or ischemia of any kind. Because neurons need blood, when the blood supply is cut off to neurons or when there's a bleed in the brain subsequent to that, often there are challenges in maintaining focus.

Andrew Huberman:

This is very common for people that have done sports where there's a lot of running into each other with your head, like rugby, football, hockey and so forth. But also people who have experienced head blows, or often overlooked is the fact that most traumatic head injury is not actually from sports, even football. It's from things like construction work ,from high impact work of that kind. So there does seem to be some efficacy of Noopept and piracetam and things like it. It's an emerging area, and as I mentioned, in the U.S., these things are sold over-the-counter. Again, you have to figure out if it's right for you, but they are beginning to show some promise. And I'm intrigued by them because of the way that they tap into the cholinergic system, which is both directly involved in focus and the ability to focus, but is also important for things related to age-related cognitive decline.

Andrew Huberman:

So a decline in cholinergic transmission or acetylcholine, as we call it, in the brain, is one of the things associated with cognitive decline. And it does seem that increasing cholinergic transmission can offset some of that cognitive decline and perhaps even more so in conditions such as vascular damage or concussion to the brain.

Andrew Huberman:

If you're interested in atypical treatments for ADHD compounds to improve focus and related themes, and you like reading about this stuff, there's an excellent review article that I can refer you to. It's by Ahn et al, A-H-N. It was published in 2016, so it's a little bit behind the times, although it's surprisingly comprehensive, given that, which lines up all the various drugs that I've discussed, racetams and Adderall and Ritalin and various forms of dopaminergic agents and cholinergic agents. Spells out whether or not they are sold over-the-counter or by prescription and really lines them up and all their effects, their drawbacks, et cetera.

Andrew Huberman:

I'll refer you to that study. It's available in its full-length form online for free. It's Ahn et al. The journal is Neural Plasticity, Neural Plasticity, 2016; should be very easy to find if you put those keywords in. And while it is a review, it is a very comprehensive review. And if you're really into this stuff, and you also want to learn a thing or two about how these things interact with neurofeedback, et cetera, there's some information in there as well. I know I've already covered a lot of information, but there is one more category of technology for the treatment of ADHD and for enhancement of focus in anyone, that I would like to emphasize. And that's transcranial magnetic stimulation. Transcranial magnetic stimulation, also called TMS, is achieving increasing popularity nowadays for the treatment of all sorts of neurologic conditions and psychiatric conditions. It is a noninvasive tool.

Andrew Huberman:

It involves taking a coil, it's a device with a coil that's placed over particular locations in the brain, and then sends magnetic stimulation into the brain. It can actually pass through the skull without having to drill through the skull. And nowadays can be used to both lower the amount of activity, or increase the amount of activity in specific brain areas. It's spatial precision is not remarkable. That doesn't mean it's not of use, but it is not a super fine-grain tool. Okay? It's not a cannon, but it's also not a needle. It is somewhere in between. It can direct the activity of particular brain regions at particular depths. And as I mentioned, it can increase or decrease that activity. So for instance, I've had a TMS coil placed on my head, not for therapeutic purposes. Even if it was I wouldn't tell you, but rather just for, well, I'm a neuroscientist and I worked in a lab with one, for entertainment, exploratory, purposes.

Andrew Huberman:

Please don't do this at home. It was placed over my motor cortex, which generates voluntary action. And it was a coil that at that time could only inhibit neurons. And so what I was doing is I was moving objects around on a table just like I am now. It was actually a pencil, not a pen, and I was tapping the pencil and then the TMS coil was turned on. And for the life of me, I could not move that pencil. Okay? Because it was inhibiting my upper motor neurons in the portion of my cortex that controls voluntary activity. As soon as the coil was turned off, I could return to tapping the pencil again. Nowadays, it's possible to stimulate motor cortex ,or any area of the brain, with some degree of precision. That could create the impulse to move without actually making the decision to move.

Andrew Huberman:

So you can literally engage certain neural circuits and therefore behaviors, and certain thought and emotional patterns, by way of transcranial magnetic stimulation. This has far reaching and vast implications, as you can probably imagine. In discussing ADHD with a colleague that uses TMS, what they are doing is they are taking the TMS coil to children and adults that have ADHD. And they're using it to stimulate the portions of the prefrontal cortex, that we talked about earlier, that engage task-directed focused states. So rather than using a drug that generally increases dopamine and some of the other chemicals involved, they're using directed TMS stimulation of these circuits. And fortunately, I was quite relieved to hear this, they're combining that with a focused learning task. So they're literally teaching the brain to learn in a noninvasive way, no drug at all. And right now there are experiments, clinical trials going on comparing TMS of this sort to the drug treatments of this sort that we described earlier that engage these circuits through pharmacologic mechanisms.

Andrew Huberman:

So very exciting times for TMS, very exciting times for pharmacology related to ADHD and for enhancing focus in general. And when I say "very exciting times,' I mean no drug is perfect, but the constellation of drugs that's out there is getting much larger. But because they tap into different aspects of their circuitry, I do think that we are well on our way to identifying the ideal combinations of drug treatments, technological treatments and behavioral paradigms for increasing focus in both children and adults with ADHD. And as a final, final point, I also want to mention something about technologies that are making it harder for all of us to focus, regardless of whether or not we have preexisting ADHD or not. You can probably guess where this is going. Everybody nowadays seems to have a smartphone. I'm sure there are a few individuals out there that don't have a smartphone.

Andrew Huberman:

Nonetheless, most people have them. Most kids want one as soon as they can get them, and they are small. They grab our attention entirely. But within that small box of attention, there are millions of attentional windows scrolling by. Right? So just because it's one device that we look at does not mean that we are focused. We are focused on our phone. But because of the way in which context switches up so fast within the phone, it's thought that the brain is struggling now to leave that rapid turnover of context. Right? Many, many shows, many, many Instagram pages, many, many Twitter feeds, many, many websites. Basically, the whole world, at least in virtual format, is available within that small box. Unlike any other technology humans have ever dealt with before. Even though there are trillions, infinite number of bits of information in the actual physical world, your attentional window, that aperture of constriction, and dilating that visual window is the way in which you cope with all that overwhelming information typically.

Andrew Huberman:

Well within the phone, your visual aperture is set to a given width. It's about this big typically. The phones seem to be getting bigger, but nonetheless, it's about that big. And within there, your attentional window is grabbing at near infinite number of bits of information, colors, movies. If a picture is worth a thousand words, a movie is worth a billion pictures. That brain loves visual motion. And so the question is, does that sort of interaction on a regular basis lead to deficits in the types of attention that we need in order to perform well in work and school relationships, et cetera? And the short answer is yes. It does appear so. We are inducing a sort of ADHD. And while the studies on this are ongoing, because prominent use of smartphones really took off right around 2010 and we're only in 2021, longstanding studies take time, which is essentially to say the same thing as longstanding.

Andrew Huberman:

There are some studies, and one in particular that I'd like to highlight. One was actually carried out pretty early in 2014. This is a study that explored smartphone use, at the time, they called it "mobile phone use," but smartphone use and inattention, difficulties in attending, in 7,102 adolescents. So this is a huge study, a population-based cross-sectional study, and you will be probably surprised and somewhat dismayed to hear that in order to avoid this decrease in attentional capacity, adolescents needed to use their smartphone for less than 60 minutes per day in order to stay focused and centered on their other tasks. Otherwise, they started to really run into significant issues. So 60 minutes is not much. I have a feeling that most young people are using their phone more than 60 minutes per day. I know I am. I think for adults the number is probably higher.

Andrew Huberman:

Meaning if you're an adult, I could, I'm going to just extrapolate from what I read in this study. It seems that probably two hours a day on the phone would be the upper limit beyond which you would probably experience pretty severe attentional deficits. I am a big fan of Cal Newport, who wrote the book "Deep Work." He's also written the excellent book "A World Without Email." I've never met him, but I'm a huge admirer of his work. And I will paraphrase something that he said far more eloquently than I ever could. Which is that the brain does not do well with constant context switching. Meaning it can do it, but it diminishes our capacity to do meaningful work of any other kind. And so Cal, as I understand — he's a computer science professor at Georgetown, by the way — is very structured and very disciplined in his avoidance of cell phone use.

Andrew Huberman:

I think we're all striving to do that. I'm not here to tell you what to do, but I think whether or not you have ADHD or not, if you're an adolescent, limiting your smartphone use to 60 minutes per day or less, and if you are an adult to two hours per day or less, is going to be among the very best ways to maintain. Just to maintain your ability to focus at whatever level you can now. And as I always say, most of the things that we get recognized for in life, success in life, in every endeavor, whether or not it's school, relationships, sport, creative works of any kind, are always proportional to the amount of focus that we can bring that activity. It is important to rest, of course, to get proper sleep. But I stand behind that statement, and I leave you with that study about attention and cell phones and how cell phones are indeed eroding our attentional capacities.

Andrew Huberman:

So I realize I covered a lot of information about ADHD and the biology of focus and how to get better at focusing. We talked about the behavioral and psychological phenotypes of ADHD. We talked about the underlying neural circuitry. We also talked about the neurochemistry, and we talked about the various prescription drug treatments that are aimed at that neurochemistry and aimed at increasing focus in children and adults with ADHD. We also talked about over-the-counter compounds, the role of particular types of diets and elimination diets. And we talked about interactions between these various features in dictating outcomes for ADHD and enhancing focus in general. We also talked a little bit about emerging neuro technologies and how certain technologies like the smartphone are no doubt hindering our ability to focus and put us at greater risk of developing ADHD at all ages. I do acknowledge the irony and, somewhat, the contradiction of doing a two-hour-plus episode on ADHD if indeed people who are watching this have challenges with attention. I want to emphasize that this podcast, like all of our podcast episodes are time stamped for a specific reason. They are designed to be digested in whatever batch one chooses. Right? You don't have to watch or listen to the entire thing all at once. However, if you've gotten to this point in the podcast, I want to thank you. I do hope that you've learned a lot about this condition. I hope you've also learned a lot about your own capacity to focus and things that you can do to enhance your focus.

Andrew Huberman:

We even talked about a tool that takes just one 17-minute session to enhance your ability to focus thereafter, presumably forever. If you're enjoying this podcast and you're learning from it, please subscribe to our YouTube channel. That really helps us. In addition, in the comment section on YouTube, you can leave us suggestions for future podcast guests and suggestions for future podcast topics that we may have not covered, or that you'd like to see covered in the future.

Andrew Huberman:

In addition, please subscribe to the podcast on Apple and Spotify. And on Apple, you have the opportunity to leave us a comment and up to a five-star review. In addition, please check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning of the podcast. That's a terrific way to support us. And for those of you that would like to support research on stress, neurobiology and human performance, you can go to hubermanlab.stanford.edu and there you can make a tax-deductible donation for research on neurobiology in my laboratory. And finally, I want to thank you for your time and your attention. And as always, thank you for your interest in science.

No items found.

Join 500,000+ subscribers to get regular emails on neuroscience, health, and science-related tools from Dr. Andrew Huberman.

You'll also get Andrew's exclusive Daily Blueprint. In it, Andrew shares his daily routine. He also shares practical tools and protocols that you can use to stay productive and maximize your health.

Thank you! Your submission has been received!
Oops! Something went wrong while submitting the form.

By submitting your email to subscribe, you agree to Scicomm Media's Privacy Policy

The Daily Blueprint

Improve your physical and mental health with Andrew’s Daily Blueprint

This comprehensive guide covers everything from morning routines and exercise to sleep optimization and stress management, helping you create a balanced, healthy lifestyle.

You’ll also join 500,000+ others in receiving Huberman Lab’s monthly Neural Network newsletter on neuroscience, health, and science-related tools from Dr. Andrew Huberman. By submitting your email to subscribe, you agree to Scicomm Media's Privacy Policy.
Thank you! Your submission has been received!
Oops! Something went wrong while submitting the form.